First: Get the data!
## R code to get data and make plots ## You may have to install the devtools and RCurl packages first library('devtools') source_gist("https://gist.github.com/richarddmorey/5f22fc742535d078c25f")
The code will also regenerate the panels from Neyman et al.'s Figure 2.
Project Whitetop
Project Whitetop, a weather modification experiment performed during the summers in 1960-1964, was one of the first meticulously randomized, large-scale experiments of its kind. Before the experiment began, every day was designated as a "seed" day or a "non-seed" day (control). The designation was kept secret until the last moment. Every day in the morning, the experimenters would determine whether the conditions were good for seeding. If there were westerly winds and "high precipitable water" in Little Rock, Arkansas and Columbia, Missouri, then the day was designated as an "experimental" day, and the envelope containing the seed instructions was opened. On seeded days, an airplane dumped silver iodide into the clouds around West Plains, Missouri. The area at the center of the concentric circles in the figure below shows the experimental area.
The original analysis as reported by Neyman et al (1969) was of the change in precipitation in the hour when the seeded plume was overhead. There appeared to be an unexpected decrease in the precipitation due to the seeding ("some" p<0.01, as it was reported by Neyman et al).
Neyman's team was interested in assessing the effect of seeding at longer time scales (24 hours) and at greater distances (up to 180 miles from the experimental area). In their minds, these longer-term, larger-range effects were much more interesting from a policy perspective.
What did Neyman think makes a good analysis?
In the same year, Neyman, Scott, and Wells wrote a paper outlining statistical inference for weather modification experiments ("Statistics in meteorology", 1969). The paper is important because it lays out what we might expect from Neyman's Whitetop analysis. They describe power as related to the notion of an "informative" experiment, and briefly mention the "optimal" class of tests that will be used to analyse the Whitetop data. The critical role of power is emphasized:
[The] rational planning of a rain stimulation experiment must emphasize the question whether, with this design, with this proposed duration and with this particular statistical test, the probability of detecting the effect of treatment that one wishes to detect is 0.2, or 0.5, or 0.8, etc. In other words, in experimentation with weather control, it is of paramount importance to estimate the power of the statistical test to be used on the data that may be provided by the contemplated design of the experiment. (p. 123)And again on page 124: "[T]he power of the test to be used in the evaluation of a rain stimulation experiment is of prime importance." This makes it explicit: the optimality of the test is interesting, but the power of the test with reference to the experimental design is critical. An optimal test can give an uninformative experiment, if the design is bad (e.g., low sample size). It should not be a surprise that Neyman emphasizes the importance of ensuring the test is worthwhile before one undertakes it. This is especially critical if one faces interpreting null results ($p>\alpha$), as Neyman points out elsewhere.
Neyman et al's Whitetop analysis
Here we discuss Neyman et al. (1969)'s analysis ("Areal Spread of the Effect of Cloud Seeding at the Whitetop Experiment"). In order to examine the effect of distance, they decided to use data from the 174 rain gages within 180 miles of the experimental area. The goal, according to Neyman et al, was two-fold:
Specifically, an effort was made to determine (i) the differences in the 24-hour precipitation amounts at different distances from the center of the Whitetop target, averaged over the 102 days with seeding and over the 96 experimental days without seeding, and (ii) the probability (P) of obtaining such differences, or larger, purely through unavoidable chance variation. (pp. 1445-1446)Of critical interest to use is how P (the p value) is interpreted later on. The figure above (overlaid on the map) shows how Neyman et al. divided the area into 6 concentric rings. For each ring A-F, the change in percent change precipitation was computed, along with a two-tailed p value. Note that a positive change was expected, and only the discovery of the negative effect in this same data set would lead one to look for a negative effect.
Before we look at the analysis results themselves, let me emphasize that I am not concerned with whether Neyman et al are correct; what I'm interested in is how they use statistics to support their case. Of special interest are some ideas that never appear in this paper. These include:
- An $\alpha$ level
- Power
- Error rates
- A significant or nonsignificant result (other than the already-mentioned, previously-published $p<.01$ result with the same data, which is described as "significant")
- Pre-determined decisions
Recreation of top panel from Neyman et al's (1969) Figure 2. See bottom panel for original caption. |
Neyman et al refer primarily to the results for all days. This is how Neyman et al describe the results:
The estimate of the average seeding effect in the entire region is a 21-percent loss of rain. In the absence of a real effect, chance alone could produce such an estimate of loss, or a larger one, about once in 15 independent trials. (p 1447)Note the switch from a two-sided p value (p=0.13, lower panel) to a one-sided p value! This occurs without any a priori mention of this hypothesis, aside from the fact that it was found in these same data previously; no mention of any pre-determined decision criterion. In fact, they note that this magnitude of negative effect is supported by no "intelligible theory" (p. 1447). This is a purely evidential use of the one-sided p value. They continue:
From the point of view of the question as to whether the current state of weather modification technology justifies its use for alleviating water shortages, [the data] appear decisive. As already mentioned, the Whitetop experiment was conducted in a locality where summer precipitation is critical. In fact, the possibilities of increases due to seeding as modest as 5 to 10 percent have been mentioned as something to be hoped for. When instead of such gains the experimental results show losses averaging 20 percent over an area of some 100,000 square miles, then even the slightest possibility that these losses were caused by seeding must be considered as disqualifying the underlying technology. Actually, the evidence in support of the causal relation between seeding and loss of rain appears quite strong. (p 1447; emphasis mine)The only mention of a decision is related to the evidence -- that is, it is decisive -- not any decision pre-planned decision. Moreover, the interpetation is concerned with the possibility, on a graded scale, that the effect is real and negative, and the evidence is "quite strong" -- again, a graded way of referring to the strength of the evidence.
Almost immediately, Battan (1969) responded to Neyman et al saying what probably most of the readers of this blog post were thinking, on seeing the mediocre p values Neyman et al used as evidence:
The two-tailed significance levels in the two tables are not so small as to make it self-evident that the rainfall differences were caused by seeding. Several hypotheses might be offered to explain effects of seeding downwind of the seeding area, but no plausible hypothesis has been offered to explain effects upwind and to the side to distances of 180 miles. (p. 618)In the clearest sign that their use of statistical inference was evidential and post-data, rather than decision-based and pre-data, Neyman et al responded:
Battan is certainly entitled to his opinion that "significance levels . . . are not so small as to make it self-evident that the rainfall differences were caused by seeding." In fact, we agree about the lack of self-evidence. But, if there is anything in the contention that a gain in the rainfall of 5 to 10 percent is worth talking about, then a 20 percent loss, experienced over a vast area of some 100,000 square miles, must be a disaster. In these conditions, the odds of 14 to 1 that this loss was caused by seeding do not appear negligible to us. We feel that it is imperative that the general public and the government be informed of the situation. (p. 618, emphasis mine)In a stunning misuse of statistics, Neyman et al have confused a p value with a posterior odds. Not just any p value; this was a p value from a post hoc one-tailed test. The need for a post-data mode of statistical inference is so great that Neyman -- who was famed for his pre-data theory of statistical inference -- is forced into a basic fallacy when responding to a critic. To me, this is quite remarkable.
Wrapping up
Fisher and Neyman disagreed about the philosophy of statistical inference. To Fisher (and indeed to almost all scientists), statistical inference was post-data and evidential. Neyman, however, had a pre-data, behavioural view of statistical inference. Neyman's viewpoint (which, unfortunately, has stuck around a long time in training of scientists using Type I and Type II errors) is not one that is conducive to science. Neyman himself, when doing science, appears to have had a post-data mind-set.
In part 3 of the series, I will look at the aftermath of the Neyman's Whitetop analysis, and how Neyman eventually abandoned the conclusions.
Your part 2 has good independent reading essay for student. I also use this blog help when i write an essay on science topic.
ReplyDeleteThis text can be read for a long time.
ReplyDeleteAriana
This comment has been removed by the author.
ReplyDeleteI like the content and details Find
ReplyDelete
ReplyDeleteI was diagnosed as HEPATITIS B carrier in 2013 with fibrosis of the
liver already present. I started on antiviral medications which
reduced the viral load initially. After a couple of years the virus
became resistant. I started on HEPATITIS B Herbal treatment from
ULTIMATE LIFE CLINIC (www.ultimatelifeclinic.com) in March, 2020. Their
treatment totally reversed the virus. I did another blood test after
the 6 months long treatment and tested negative to the virus. Amazing
treatment! This treatment is a breakthrough for all HBV carriers.
Yes, I am entirely agreed with this article, and I just want say that this article is very helpful and enlightening. I also have some precious piece of concerned info !!!!!!Thanks. EHR Software
ReplyDeletehowdy, your websites are really good. I appreciate your work. Angular trainer
ReplyDeletelinkedin tool Wow, cool post. I'd like to write like this too - taking time and real hard work to make a great article... but I put things off too much and never seem to get started. Thanks though.
ReplyDelete